HIV Articles  
Back 
 
 
The persistence of HIV-associated neurocognitive
dysfunction and the effects of comorbidities - Editorial
 
 
  Neurology Dec 7 2010
 
"47% of the sample without major comorbidities met criteria for HAND......That a significant association between neurocognitive impairment and HIV disease variables was observed only for the individuals with minimal ("incidental") comorbidities is particularly interesting. Two possibilities may explain this finding. First, it is possible that the influence of HIV, though significant on its own, is relatively small in the context of the many comorbid conditions that can affect the brain, such as substance use, traumatic brain injury, and stroke. Additionally, a synergy between HIV infection and these comorbid conditions may exist which could amplify their impact on brain functioning (from Jules: immunosenescence!......possible explanation is the potential role of HIV disease history on current neurocognitive function, as evident here by the relationship between history of immune suppression (nadir CD4) and neurocognitive function. Additionally, nadir CD4 and duration of infection have been found to predict HIV-associated cerebral atrophylongitudinal studies are needed to track progression of neurocognitive function over time relative to other measures of brain health. Finally, given the apparent importance of comorbid conditions, the current study should also serve as a call for more intensive investigation of the assessment and treatment of hepatitis C, substance abuse, and other comorbidities in the context of HIV."
 
Ronald A. Cohen, PhD, ABPP, ABCN and Assawin Gongvatana, PhD From the Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University, Providence, RI.
 
Despite the effectiveness of combination antiretroviral therapy (CART), many HIV-infected patients continue to experience cognitive problems.1 The study by Heaton et al.2 in this issue of Neurology examined HIV-associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND) in a large national cohort from the multicenter CHARTER study, which assesses the changes in presentation of HIV neurologic complications in the modern era of HIV treatment. Only 2% of the cohort exhibited dementia, confirming clinical observations that severe functional decline is now relatively rare.1 Yet 47% of the sample without major comorbidities met criteria for HAND. It is notable that the latter prevalence is similar to those reported in studies conducted before modern antiretroviral therapy.3 It is likely that this is partly attributable to the fact that HIV-infected patients are living longer with effective treatment, such that aging contributes to the prevalence of neurocognitive deficits. Nevertheless, the results provide clear evidence that HAND continues to be common despite the effectiveness of CART, and the prevalence of neurocognitive impairment is even greater when factoring in people with significant comorbidities commonly experienced by HIV-infected patients.
 
The study has a number of merits that make it likely to be considered a seminal investigation of HAND in the current era. The study employed a comprehensive set of validated neuropsychological tests with well-established norms. Diagnosis of HAND was conducted based on the up-to-date and widely accepted Frascati criteria.4 The significance of the findings is also bolstered by the large sample size (n = 1,555) obtained from multiple centers, providing a representative cross-section of HIV-infected individuals in the United States. The use of a classification system for rating clinical comorbidities and their potential neurocognitive contributions4 is another strength of the study. Accordingly, 15% of the sample was considered to have severe comorbidities that preclude a HAND diagnosis. The study employed 2 raters to establish the reliability of the comorbidity classification, which is a methodologic strength, and would be recommended for future studies that employ similar classification systems.
 
That a significant association between neurocognitive impairment and HIV disease variables was observed only for the individuals with minimal ("incidental") comorbidities is particularly interesting. Two possibilities may explain this finding. First, it is possible that the influence of HIV, though significant on its own, is relatively small in the context of the many comorbid conditions that can affect the brain, such as substance use, traumatic brain injury, and stroke. Additionally, a synergy between HIV infection and these comorbid conditions may exist which could amplify their impact on brain functioning, but which may not be apparent without explicit analysis of their statistical interactions. These 2 possibilities cannot be reconciled based on results from the current study, so additional studies are needed to address this question.
 
The importance of neurologic conditions such as stroke, traumatic brain injury, and seizure disorder in classifying the level of comorbid confounds is relatively obvious. While mounting evidence suggests that other conditions, including hepatitis C and substance abuse,5 affect neurocognitive function, the mechanisms underlying these effects in conjunction with HIV are not well-understood. Furthermore, is not clear how the confounding impact of such factors can be judged, nor is it obvious that these comorbidities should be viewed as a basis for exclusion from a HAND diagnosis, given their importance to the clinical phenomena of HIV. Although the authors provide evidence of good interrater reliability of the comorbidity classification system, it should be noted that this is distinct from a demonstration of the validity of the classification, which is dependent on the scientific accuracy of the guideline for the classification system. It is therefore important that such guidelines be adjusted as our knowledge of these confounding conditions advances.
 
The question remains as to why neurocognitive deficits remain prevalent in HIV-infected people despite immunologic and virologic controls afforded by CART. Inadequate penetration of CART into the CNS has been considered as a possible reason,6 though findings from the current study did not support this hypothesis, so future longitudinal analysis of this cohort is needed to clarify this issue. Another possible explanation is the potential role of HIV disease history on current neurocognitive function, as evident here by the relationship between history of immune suppression (nadir CD4) and neurocognitive function. Additionally, nadir CD4 and duration of infection have been found to predict HIV-associated cerebral atrophy.7
 
Additional direct measures of the brain are necessary to further examine the etiology of HAND. Recent advances in MRI in conjunction with brain metabolite measures via magnetic resonance spectroscopy have considerable potential this regard,7,-,9 and may ultimately provide biomarkers sensitive to neurocognitive and functional outcome. Furthermore, longitudinal studies are needed to track progression of neurocognitive function over time relative to other measures of brain health. Finally, given the apparent importance of comorbid conditions, the current study should also serve as a call for more intensive investigation of the assessment and treatment of hepatitis C, substance abuse, and other comorbidities in the context of HIV.
 
DISCLOSURE
 
Dr. Cohen serves on the editorial boards of the Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, The Clinical Neuropsychologist, and Brain Imaging and Behavior; is funded by the NIH (R01 MH074368 [PI], HL084178-01 [coinvestigator], NS036524 and S1 [coinvestigator], DK075119 [coinvestigator], and R01HL089311 [coinvestigator]); and receives royalties from the publication of Neuropsychology of Attention (Springer, 1993). Dr. Gongvatana reports no disclosures.
 
Footnotes
 
Supported by the National Institutes of Health.
See page 2087.
 
REFERENCES
 
1. McArthur JC. HIV dementia: an evolving disease. J Neu- roimmunol 2004;157:3-10.
 
2. Heaton RK, Clifford DB, Franklin DR Jr, et al. HIV- associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of po- tent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER Study. Neurology 2010;75:2087-2096.
 
3. Heaton RK, Grant I, Butters N, et al. The HNRC 500- neuropsychology of HIV infection at different disease stages: HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center. J Int Neu- ropsychol Soc 1995;1:231-251.
 
4. Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, et al. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology 2007;69:1789 -1799.
 
5. Martin-Thormeyer EM, Paul RH. Drug abuse and hepati- tis C infection as comorbid features of HIV associated neu- rocognitive disorder: neurocognitive and neuroimaging features. Neuropsychol Rev 2009;19:215-231.
 
6. Cysique LA, Vaida F, Letendre S, et al. Dynamics of cog- nitive change in impaired HIV-positive patients initiating antiretroviral therapy. Neurology 2009;73:342-348.
 
7. Cohen RA, Harezlak J, Schifitto G, et al. Effects of nadir CD4 count and duration of human immunodeficiency virus infection on brain volumes in the highly active antiretroviral therapy era. Journal of neurovirology 2010;16:25-32.
 
8. Gongvatana A, Schweinsburg BC, Taylor MJ, et al. White matter tract injury and cognitive impairment in human immunodeficiency virus-infected individuals. J Neurovirol 2009;15:187-195.
 
9. Paul RH, Ernst T, Brickman AM, et al. Relative sensitivity of magnetic resonance spectroscopy and quantitative mag- netic resonance imaging to cognitive function among non- demented individuals infected with HIV. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14:725-733.
 
 
 
 
  iconpaperstack view older Articles   Back to Top   www.natap.org